home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- >>>alt/paranormal 71 karttu@mits.mdata.fi(27404)14Jun92 01:18
- TITLE: Lucid Dreaming, article by Susan Blackmore from SI, Vol 15 Summer 1991
-
- +Message-ID : <1992Jun14.011826.7836@mits.mdata.fi>
- +From : karttu@mits.mdata.fi (Antti Karttunen)
- +Organization : MITS, Helsinki, Finland
-
- In article <14334@ucdavis.ucdavis.edu> shaw@toadflax.UCDavis.EDU (Rob Shaw) writes:
- >Let's not neglect lucid dreams as an altered states.
-
-
-
- LUCID DREAMING: AWAKE IN YOUR SLEEP?
-
- By Susan Blackmore
-
- From Skeptical Inquirer, Vol. 15 Summer 1991
- pages 362-370
-
-
- What could it mean to be conscious in your dreams? For most
- of us, dreaming is something quite separate from normal life.
- When we wake up from being chased by a ferocious tiger, or
- seduced by a devastatingly good-looking Nobel Prize winner
- we realize with relief or disappointment that "it was only
- a dream."
- Yet there are some dreams that are not like that. Lucid
- dreams are dreams in which you know at the time that you are
- dreaming. That they are different from ordinary dreams is
- obvious as soon as you have one. The experience is something
- like waking up in your dreams. It is as though you "come to"
- and find you are dreaming.
- Lucid dreams used to be a topic within psychical research
- and parapsychology. Perhaps their incomprehensibility made
- them good candidates for being thought paranormal. More
- recently, however, they have begun to appear in psychology
- journals and have dropped out of parapsychology - a good
- example of how the field of parapsychology shrinks when any
- of its subject matter is actually explained.
- Lucidity has also become something of a New Age fad. There
- are machines and gadgets you can buy and special clubs you
- can join to learn how to induce lucid dreams. But this
- commercialization should not let us lose sight of the very
- real fascination of lucid dreaming. It forces us to ask
- questions about the nature of consciousness, deliberate
- control over our actions, and the nature of imaginary worlds.
-
- A Real Dream or Not?
-
- The term lucid dreaming was coined by the Dutch psychiatrist
- Frederik van Eeden in 1913. It is something of a misnomer
- since it means something quite different from just clear or
- vivid dreaming. Nevertheless we are certainly stuck with it.
- Van Eeden explained that in this sort of dream "the
- re-integration of the psychic functions is so complete that
- the sleeper reaches a state of perfect awareness and is able
- to direct his attention, and to attempt different acts of free
- volition. Yet the sleep, as I am able confidently to state, is
- undisturbed, deep, and refreshing."
- This implied that there could be consciousness during sleep,
- a claim many psychologists denied for more than 50 years.
- Orthodox sleep researchers argued that lucid dreams could not
- possibly be real dreams. If the accounts were valid, then the
- experiences must have occurred during brief moments of
- wakefulness or in the transition between waking and sleeping,
- not in the kind of deep sleep in which rapid eye movements (REMs)
- and ordinary dreams usually occur. In other words, they could
- not really be dreams at all.
- This presented a challenge to lucid dreamers who wanted to
- convince people that they really were awake in their dreams.
- But of course when you are deep asleep and dreaming you cannot
- shout, "Hey! Listen to me. I'm dreaming right now." All the
- muscles of the body are paralyzed.
- It was Keith Hearne (1978), of the University of Hull, who
- first exploited the fact that not all the muscles are paralyzed.
- In REM sleep the eyes move. So perhaps a lucid dreamer could
- signal by moving the eyes in a predetermined pattern. Just over
- ten years ago, lucid dreamer Alan Worsley first managed this is
- in Hearne's laboratory. He decided to move his eyes left and
- right eight times in succession whenever he became lucid. Using
- a polygraph, Hearne could watch the eye movements for sign of
- the special signal. He found it in the midst of REM sleep. So
- lucid dreams are real dreams and do occur during REM sleep.
- Further research showed that Worsley's lucid dreams most
- often occurred in the early morning, around 6:30 A.M., nearly
- half an hour into a REM period and toward the end of a burst
- of rapid eye movements. They usually lasted for two to five
- minutes. Later research showed that they occur at times of
- particularly high arousal during REM sleep (Hearne 1978).
- It is sometimes said that discoveries in science happen when
- the time is right for them. It was one of those odd things that
- at just the same time, but unbeknown to Hearne, Stephen LaBerge,
- at Stanford University in California, was trying the same
- experiment. He too succeeded, but resistance to the idea was
- very strong. In 1980, both Science and Nature rejected his first
- paper on the discovery (LaBerge 1985). It was only later that it
- became clear what an important step this had been.
-
- An Identifiable State?
-
- It would be especially interesting if lucid dreams were
- associated with a unique physiological state. In fact this has
- not been found, although this is not very surprising since the
- same is true of other altered states, such as out-of-body
- experiences and trances of various kinds. However, lucid dreams
- do tend to occur in periods of higher cortical arousal. Perhaps
- a certain threshold of arousal has to be reached before awareness
- can be sustained.
- The beginning of lucidity (marked by eye signals, of course)
- is associated with pauses in breathing, brief changes in heart
- rate, and skin response changes, but there is no unique
- combination that allows the lucidity to be identified by an
- observer.
- In terms of the dream itself, there are several features that
- seem to provoke lucidity. Sometimes heightened anxiety or stress
- precedes it. More often there is a kind of intellectual
- recognition that something "dreamlike" or incongruous is going
- on (Fox 1962; Green 1968; LaBerge 1985).
- It is common to wake from an ordinary dream and wonder, "How
- on earth could I have been fooled into thinking that I was
- really doing push-ups on a blue beach?" A little more awareness
- is shown when we realize this in the dream. If you ask yourself,
- "Could this be a dream?" and answer "No" (or don't answer at all),
- this is called a pre-lucid dream. Finally, if you answer "Yes",
- it becomes a fully lucid dream.
- It could be that once there is sufficient cortical arousal it
- is possible to apply a bit of critical thought; to remember
- enough about how the world ought to be to recognize the dream
- world as ridiculous, or perhaps to remember enough about oneself
- to know that these events can't be continuous with normal waking
- life. However, tempting as it is to conclude that the critical
- insight produces the lucidity, we have only an apparent
- correlation and cannot deduce cause and effect from it.
-
- Becoming a Lucid Dreamer
-
- Surveys have show that about 50 percent of people (and in some
- cases more) have had at least one lucid dream in their lives.
- (see, for example, Blackmore 1982; Gackenbach and LaBerge 1988;
- Green 1968.) Of course surveys are unreliable in that many
- people may not understand the question. In particular, if you
- have never had a lucid dream, it is easy to misunderstand what
- is meant by the term. So overestimates might be expected.
- Beyond this, it does not seem that surveys can find out much.
- There are no very consistent differences between lucid dreamers
- and others in terms of age, sex, education, and so on (Green
- 1968; Gackenbach and LaBerge 1988).
- For many people, having lucid dream is fun, and they want to
- learn how to have more or to induce them at will. One finding
- from early experimental work was that high levels of physical
- (and emotional) activity during the day tend to precede lucidity
- at night. Waking during the night and carrying out some kind of
- activity before falling asleep again can also encourage a lucid
- dream during the next REM period and is the basis of some
- induction techniques.
- Many methods have been developed (Gackenbach and Bosveld 1989;
- Tart 1988; Price and Cohen 1988). They roughly fall into three
- categories.
- One of the best known is LaBerge's MILD (Mnemonic Induction
- of Lucid Dreaming). This is done on waking in the early morning
- from a dream. You should wake up fully, engage in some activity
- like reading or walking about, and then lie down to go to sleep
- again. Then you must imagine yourself asleep and dreaming,
- rehearse the dream from which you woke, and remind yourself,
- "Next time I dream this I want to remember I'm dreaming."
- A second approach involves constantly reminding yourself to
- become lucid throughout the day rather than the night. This is
- based on the idea that we spend most of our time in a kind of
- waking daze. If we could be more lucid in waking life, perhaps
- we could be more lucid while dreaming. German psychologist
- Paul Tholey suggests asking yourself many times every day,
- "Am I dreaming or not?" This sound easy but is not. It takes
- a lot of determination and persistence not to forget all about
- it. For those who do forget, French researcher Clerc suggests
- writing a large "C" on your hand (for "conscious") to remind
- you (Tholey 1983; Gackenbach and Bosveld 1989).
- This kind of method is similar to the age-old technique for
- increasing awareness by meditation and mindfulness. Advanced
- practitioners of meditation claim to maintain awareness through
- a large proportion of their sleep. TM is often claimed to lead
- to sleep awareness. So perhaps it is not surprising that some
- recent research finds association between meditation and
- increased lucidity (Gackenbach and Bosveld 1989).
- The third and final approach requires a variety of gadgets.
- The idea is to use some sort of external signal to remind
- people, while they are actually in REM sleep, that they are
- dreaming. Hearne first tried spraying water onto sleepers' faces
- or hands but found it too unreliable. This sometimes caused them
- to incorporate water imagery into their dreams, but they rarely
- became lucid. He eventually decided to use a mild electrical
- shock to the wrist. His "dream machine" detects changes in
- breathing rate (which accompany the onset of REM) and then
- automatically delivers a shock to the wrist (Hearne 1990).
- Meanwhile, in California, LaBerge was rejecting taped voices
- and vibrations and working instead with flashing lights. The
- original version was laboratory based and used a personal
- computer to detect the eye movements of REM sleep and to turn
- on flashing lights whenever the REMs reached a certain level.
- Eventually, however, all the circuitry was incorporated into
- a pair of goggles. The idea is to put the goggles on at night,
- and the lights will flash only when you are asleep and dreaming.
- The user can even control the level of eye movements at which
- the lights begin to flash.
- The newest version has a chip incorporated into the goggles.
- This will not only control the lights but will store data on
- eye-movement density during the night and when and for how long
- the lights were flashing, making fine tuning possible. At the
- moment, the first users have to join in workshops at LaBerge's
- Lucidity Institute and learn how to adjust the settings, but
- within a few months he hopes the whole process will be fully
- automated. (See LaBerge's magazine, DreamLight.)
- LaBerge tested the effectiveness of the Dream Light on 44
- subjects who came into the laboratory, most for just one night.
- Fifty-five percent had at least one lucid dream this way. The
- results suggested that this method is about as succesful as
- MILD, but using the two together is the most effective
- (LaBerge 1985).
-
- Lucid Dreams as an Experimental Tool
-
- There are a few people who can have lucid dreams at will. And
- the increase in induction techniques has provided many more
- subjects who have them frequently. This has opened the way to
- using lucid dreams to answer some of the most interesting
- questions about sleep and dreaming.
- How long do dreams take? In the last century, Alfred Maury
- had a long and complicated dream that led to his being beheaded
- by a guillotine. He woke up terrified, and found that the
- headboard of his bed had fallen on his neck. From this, the
- story goes, he concluded that the whole dream had been created
- in the moment of awakening.
- This idea seems to have got into popular folklore but was very
- hard to test. Researchers woke dreamers at various stages of
- their REM period and found that those who had been longer in REM
- claimed longer dreams. However, accurate timing became possible
- only when lucid dreamers could send "markers" from the dream
- state.
- LaBerge asked his subjects to signal when they became lucid
- and then count a ten-second period and signal again. Their
- average interval was 13 seconds, the same as they gave when
- awake. Lucid dreamers, like Alan Worsley, have also been able
- to give accurate estimates of the length of whole dreams or
- dream segments (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick 1988).
-
- Dream Actions
-
- As we watch sleeping animals it is often tempting to conclude
- that they are moving their eyes in response to watching a dream,
- or twitching their legs as they dream of chasing prey. But do
- physical movements actually relate to the dream events?
- Early sleep researchers occassionally reported examples like
- a long series of left-right eye movements when a dreamer had
- been dreaming of watching a ping-pong game, but they could do
- no more than wait until the right sort of dream came along.
- Lucid dreaming made proper experimentation possible, for the
- subjects could be asked to perform a whole range of tasks in
- their dreams. In one experiment with researchers Morton Schatzman
- and Peter Fenwick, in London, Worsley planned to draw large
- triangles and to signal with flicks of his eyes every time he
- did so. While he dreamed, the electromyogram, recording small
- muscle movements, showed not only the eye signals but spikes
- of electrical activity in the right forearm just afterward.
- This showed that the preplanned actions in the dream produced
- corresponding muscle movements (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick
- 1988).
- Further experiments, with Worsley kicking dream objects,
- writing with umbrellas, and snapping his fingers, all confirmed
- that the muscles of the body show small movements corresponding
- to the body's actions in the dream. The question about eye
- movements was also answered. The eyes do track dream objects.
- Worsley could even produce slow scanning movements, which are
- very difficult to produce in the absence of a "real" stimulus
- (Schatzman, Worsley, and Fenwick 1988).
- LaBerge was especially interested in breathing during dreams.
- This stemmed from his experiences at age five when he had
- dreamed of being an undersea pirate who could stay under water
- for very long periods without drowning. Thirty years later he
- wanted to find out whether dreamers holding their breath in
- dreams do so physically as well. The answer was yes. He and
- other lucid dreamers were able to signal from the dream and
- then hold their breath. They could also breathe rapidly in
- their dreams, as revealed on the monitors. Studying breathing
- during dreamed speech, he found that the person begins to
- breathe out at the start of an utterance just as in real speech
- (LaBerge and Dement 1982a).
-
- Hemispheric Differences
-
- It is known that the left and right hemispheres are activated
- differently during different kinds of tasks. For example,
- singing uses the right hemisphere more, while counting and
- other, more analytical tasks use the left hemisphere more. By
- using lucid dreams, LaBerge was able to find out whether the
- same is true in dreaming.
- In one dream he found himself flying over a field. (Flying
- is commonly associated with lucid dreaming.) He signaled with
- his eyes and began to sing "Row, row, row your boat...."
- He then made another signal and counted slowly to ten before
- signaling again. The brainwave records showed just the same
- patterns of activation that you would expect if he had done
- these tasks while awake (LaBerge and Dement 1982b).
-
- Dream Sex
-
- Although it is not often asked experimentally, I am sure plenty
- of people have wondered what is happening in their bodies while
- they have their most erotic dreams.
- LaBerge tested a woman who could dream lucidly at will and
- could direct her dreams to create the sexual experiences she
- wanted. (What a skill!) Using appropriate physiological
- recording, he was able to show that her dream orgasms were
- matched by true orgasms (LaBerge, Greenleaf, and Kedzierski
- 1983).
- Experiments like these show that there is a close
- correspondence between actions of the dreamer and, if not real
- movements, at least eletrical responses. This puts lucid
- dreaming somewhere between real actions, in which muscles work
- to move the body, and waking imagery, in which they are rarely
- involved at all. So what exactly is the status of the dream
- world?
-
- The Nature of the Dream World
-
- It is tempting to think that the real world and the world of
- dreams are totally separate. Some of the experiments already
- mentioned show that there is no absolute dividing line. There
- are also plenty of stories that show the penetrability of the
- boundary.
- Alan Worsley describes one experiment in which his task was
- to give himself a prearranged number of small electric shocks
- by means of a machine measuring his eye movements. He went to
- sleep and began dreaming that it was raining and he was in a
- sleeping bag by a fence with gate in it. He began to wonder
- whether he was dreaming and thought it would be cheating to
- activate the shocks if he was awake. Then, while making the
- signals, he worried about the machine, for it was out there
- with him in the rain and might get wet (Schatzman, Worsley,
- and Fenwick 1988).
- This kind of interference is amusing, but there are dreams
- of confusion that are not. The most common and distinct are
- called false awakenings. You dream of waking up but in fact,
- of course, are still asleep. Van Eeden (1913) called these
- "wrong waking up" and described them as "demoniacal, uncanny,
- and very vivid and bright, with ... a strong diabolical light."
- The French zoologist Yves Delage, writing in 1919, described
- how he had heard a knock at his door and a friend calling for
- his help. He jumped out of bed, went to wash quickly with cold
- water, and when that woke him up he realized he had been
- dreaming. The sequence repeated four times before he finally
- actually woke up - still in bed.
- A student of mine described her infuriating recurrent dream
- of getting up, cleaning her teeth, getting dressed, and then
- cycling all the way to the medical school at the top of a long
- hill, where she finally would realize that she had dreamed it
- all, was late for lectures, and would have to do it all over
- again for real.
- The one positive benefit of false awakenings is that they can
- sometimes be used to induce out-of-body-experiences (OBEs).
- Indeed, Oliver Fox (1962) recommends this as a method for
- achieving the OBE. For many people OBEs and lucid dreams are
- practically indistinguishable. If you dream of leaving your
- body, the experience is much the same. Also recent research
- suggests that the same people tend to have both lucid dreams
- and OBEs (Blackmore 1988, Irwin 1988).
- All of these experiences have something in common. In all
- of them the "real" wolrd has been replaced by some kind of
- imaginary replica. Celia Green, of the Institute of
- Psychophysical Research at Oxford, refers to all such states
- as "metachoric experiences."
- Jayne Gackenbach, a psychologist from the University of
- Alberta, Canada, relates these experiences to UFO-abduction
- stories and near-death-experiences (NDEs). The UFO abductions
- are the most bizarre but are similar in that they too involve
- the replacement of the perceived world by a hallucinatory
- replica.
- There is an important difference between lucid dreams and
- these other states. In the lucid dream one has insight into
- the state (in fact that defines it). In false awakening, one
- does not (again by definition). In typical OBEs, people think
- they have really left their bodies. In UFO "abductions" they
- believe the little green men are "really there"; and in NDEs,
- they are convinced they are rushing down a real tunnel toward
- a real light and into the next world. It is only in the lucid
- dream that one realizes it is a dream.
- I have often wondered whether insight into these other
- experiences is possible and what the consequences might be.
- So far I don't have any answers.
-
- Waking Up
-
- The oddest thing about lucid dreams - and, to many people who
- have them, the most compelling - is how it feels when you wake
- up. Upon waking up from a normal dream, you usually think,
- "Oh, that was only a dream." Waking up from a lucid dream is
- more continuous. It feels more real, it feels as though you
- were conscious in the dream. Why is this? I think the reason can
- be found by looking at the mental models the brain constructs
- in waking, in ordinary dreaming, and in lucid dreams.
- I have previously argued that what seems real is the most
- stable mental model in the system at any time. In waking life,
- this is almost always the input-driven model, the one that is
- built up from the sensory input. It is firmly linked to the
- body image to make a stable model of "me, here, now." It is
- easy to decide that this represents "reality" while all the
- other models being used at the same time are "just imagination"
- (Blackmore 1988).
- Now consider an ordinary dream. In that case there are lots
- of models being built but no input-driven model. In addition
- there is no adequate self-model or body image. There is just
- not enough access to memory to construct it. This means, if my
- hypothesis is right, that whatever model is most stable at any
- time will seem real. But there is no recognizable self to whom
- it seems real. There will just be a series of competing models
- coming and going. Is this what dreaming feels like?
- Finally, we know from research that in the lucid dream there
- is higher arousal. Perhaps this is sufficient to construct a
- better model of self. It is one that includes such important
- facts as that you have gone to sleep, that you intended to
- signal with your eyes, and so on. It is also more similar to
- the normal waking self than those fleeting constructions of
- the ordinary dream. This, I suggest, is what makes the dream
- seem more real on waking up. Because the you who remembers
- the dream is more similar to the you in the dream. Indeed,
- because there was a better model of you, you were more
- conscious.
- If this is right, it means that lucid dreams are potentially
- even more interesting than we thought. As well as providing
- insight into the nature of sleep and dreams, they may give
- clues to the nature of consciousness itself.
-
- References
-
-
- Blackmore, S. J. 1982. Beyond the Body. London: Heinemann.
- --------- 1988. A Theory of lucid dreams and OBEs.
- In Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain, 373-387, ed.
- J. Gackenbach and S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- Delage, Y. 1919. Le Reve. Paris: Les Presses Universitaires de
- France.
- Fox, O. 1962. Astral Projection. New York: University Books.
- Gackenbach, J., and J. Bosveld. 1989. Control Your Dreams.
- New York: Harper & Row.
- Gackenbach, J., and S. LaBerge, eds. 1988. Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain. New York: Plenum.
- Green, C. E. 1968. Lucid Dreams. London: Hamish Hamilton.
- Hearne, K. 1978. Lucid Dreams: An Electrophysiological and
- Psychological Study. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis,
- University of Hull.
- --------- 1990. The Dream Machine. Northants: Aquarian.
- Irwin, H. J. 1988. Out-of-body experiences and dream lucidity:
- Empirical perspectives. In Conscious Mind,
- Sleeping Brain, 353-371, ed. J. Gackenbach and
- S. LaBerge. New York: Plenum.
- LaBerge, S. 1985. Lucid Dreaming. Los Angeles: Tarcher.
- LaBerge, S. and W. Dement. 1982a. Voluntary control of respiration
- during REM sleep. Sleep Research, 11:107.
- --------- 1982b. Lateralization of alpha activity for dreamed
- singing and counting during REM sleep.
- Psychophysiology, 19:331-332.
- LaBerge, S., W. Greenleaf, and B. Kerzierski. 1983. Physiological
- responses to dreamed sexual activity during lucid
- REM sleep. Psychophysiology, 20:454-455.
- Price, R. F., and D. B. Cohen. 1988. Lucid dream induction: An
- empirical evaluation. In Conscious Mind, Sleeping
- Brain, 105-134, ed. J. Gackenbach and S. LaBerge.
- New York: Plenum.
- Schatzman, M., A. Worsley, and P. Fenwick. 1988. Correspondence
- during lucid dreams between dreamed and actual
- events. In Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain,
- 155-179, ed. J. Gackenbach and S. LaBerge.
- New York: Plenum.
- Tart, C. 1988. From spontaneous event to lucidity: A review of
- attempts to consciously control nocturnal
- dreaming. In Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain,
- 67-103, ed. J Gackenbach and S. LaBerge.
- New York: Plenum.
- Tholey, P. 1983. Techniques for controlling and manipulating
- lucid dreams. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
- 57:79-90.
- Van Eeden, F. 1913. A study of dreams. Proceedings of the Society
- for Psychical Research, 26:431-461.
-
- Susan J. Blackmore is with the Perceptual Systems Research Centre,
- Department of Psychology, University of Bristol, and the School
- of Social Sciences, University of Bath.
-
-
-
-
-